A Crucial Element of Democracy

This is a blog by Robert Gutierrez ...
While often taken for granted, civics education plays a crucial role in a democracy like ours. This Blog is dedicated to enticing its readers into taking an active role in the formulation of the civics curriculum found in their local schools. In order to do this, the Blog is offering a newer way to look at civics education, a newer construct - liberated federalism or federation theory. Daniel Elazar defines federalism as "the mode of political organization that unites separate polities within an overarching political system by distributing power among general and constituent governments in a manner designed to protect the existence and authority of both." It depends on its citizens acting in certain ways which Elazar calls federalism's processes. Federation theory, as applied to civics curriculum, has a set of aims. They are:
*Teach a view of government as a supra federated institution of society in which collective interests of the commonwealth are protected and advanced.
*Teach the philosophical basis of government's role as guardian of the grand partnership of citizens at both levels of individuals and associations of political and social intercourse.
*Convey the need of government to engender levels of support promoting a general sense of obligation and duty toward agreed upon goals and processes aimed at advancing the common betterment.
*Establish and justify a political morality which includes a process to assess whether that morality meets the needs of changing times while holding true to federalist values.
*Emphasize the integrity of the individual both in terms of liberty and equity in which each citizen is a member of a compacted arrangement and whose role is legally, politically, and socially congruent with the spirit of the Bill of Rights.
*Find a balance between a respect for national expertise and an encouragement of local, unsophisticated participation in policy decision-making and implementation.
Your input, as to the content of this Blog, is encouraged through this Blog directly or the Blog's email address: gravitascivics@gmail.com .
NOTE: This blog has led to the publication of a book. The title of that book is TOWARD A FEDERATED NATION: IMPLEMENTING NATIONAL CIVICS STANDARDS and it is available through Amazon in both ebook and paperback versions.

Tuesday, August 30, 2016

THE STRUCTURE AND PROCESSES OF THE MIND

[Note:  Due to the length of this essay, I will submit it over two postings.]

Continuing with a review of the different psychological schools of thought that have had an influence on education, cognitive psychology is the next approach I will address.  Whereas most school teachers and administrators are behaviorists by inclination, this next approach probably enjoys the second most favored position among those educators.  Not that any significant number of teachers and administrators wake up in the morning consciously thinking they are cognitivists, but given the general ideas attributed to this approach, I would suppose cognitive ideas probably have a fairly wide acceptance among them.

What are those ideas?  To begin with, more committed cognitivists are heavily concerned with the issues of growth or development, as opposed to learning, per se.  With this emphasis, they make a conscious differentiation between brain and mind.  With this distinction firmly established – which I will describe shortly – they contextualize the difference between development and learning.  Development is wired into the human make up; it’s a genetic trait, although it varies, in terms of its pace and its qualitative aspects, among people.  In regard to this concern, there have been famous models developed (no pun intended) by famous psychologists; that is, their names are somewhat commonly known and I will in this review describe the work of two of them.

But before we take a look at representative models (a topic for the next posting), let’s review this distinction between brain and mind.  In common parlance, many of us interchange these two terms.  But cognitivists, when they are speaking seriously about their field of study, make a distinction.  Let me describe this by using an analogy.  If you think of a radio or TV, you might think of a plastic casing in whose insides are filled with wires, transistors, and the like.  This constitutes these items’ physical attributes.  This, in the analogy, is akin to the brain.  The brain is the organ in a person’s head that takes up the relatively large area situated behind a person’s eyes, within the skull.  I’m sure we have all seen extracted brains of deceased people. 

What exactly is included and excluded from what is considered the brain might vary among those who talk about the brain, but generally we all know to what the term refers.  The brain provides the physically required mechanism by which thinking can take place.  There are portions of the brain that are associated with certain types of thinking – knowledge, beliefs, and emotions – and this passing reference is a good place to point out that cognitivists have no qualms about going inside the brain to try to figure out why we think and behave the way we do (remember, I pointed out the reluctance of behaviorists to do so).[1]

But we don’t buy radios and TVs to marvel at this complex amalgamation of wires and transistors.  Instead, we buy these appliances to be informed and entertained.  That is, spewing out of a radio or TV is programming which has its own order and logic.  This aspect of these devices is what is akin to the mind.  The mind is the sum total of all the “content” of our thinking.  The mind is also organized with structured content.  This content is not all consciously “observed” by a person; some of it is subconscious and nonconscious.  For example, we are not conscious of how the mind is genetically geared to develop at its own pace (more on this below).  Of course, all of these mind activities are dependent upon a functioning brain and we can observe in people what happens when portions of the brain are damaged and how such injuries affect thinking and, therefore, the mind.

So, cognitivists are very interested in all of this; they study how the mind organizes its thinking and the knowledge it holds and processes.  They spend a lot of time thinking about how the mind structures that knowledge and what actions it takes to hold and expand that knowledge.  Such mental processes, such as organizing, storing, discovering, creating, evaluating, and implementing content, are central to their study of psychology.  This, by necessity, expands their interest into not only the quantity of knowledge, but also the quality of knowledge.  That is, how does certain knowledge and how it is structured affect further thinking?  For example, how does something surprising or challenging to present knowledge that spurs an individual to become curious or creative or apt to solve a problem that is contained within that which the mind is considering? 

Note the avoidance of using the term stimulus – its use is not taboo, but it harkens back to those other psychologists about whom we just learned.  I might be imposing this distinction here, but my experience with this issue is that behaviorists are concerned with what is external to the mind; that is, what is the stimulus, while the cognitivists’ concern is centered on what is internal by analyzing, as best they can, the structures and processes of the mind.

The other area of concern that cognitivists are drawn to is memory.  Here, a very important distinction is made between short-term memory and long-term memory.  Short-term memory is the immediate sort of images, thoughts, connections (to prior stored information), and proclivities (such as the influence that framing has) in which the mind engages.  Much of this thinking, using short-term memory, is automatic and much of its content is a product of genetic factors.  For example, if I, out of the blue, offer you a gambling opportunity in which you put up $100 to win $150 with an 80% chance of success, you, if you are like most people, will decline the bet, not because it’s logical or reasonable to decline it, but because that’s just the way most of us are.  We are, automatically, a risk averse species.[2]  Yes, there are some who are not so averse to such bets; you know who they are, but most of us firmly believe the adage – “a bird in the hand is worth two in the bush” – as too chancy a proposition.  This proclivity is not learned; it’s just there and cognitivists can tell you what part of the brain “lights” up when considering this bet and this bias.  And short-term operations based on short-term memory deal with all of this.  It is mostly what Daniel Kahneman considers effortless thinking.[3]

He calls it System 1 thinking, but there is also System 2 thinking which is effortful.  That is, a person delves into long-term memory, the memory that is composed of knowledge that is retained for more than thirty seconds (as it is with short-term memory) and is available to apply to new situations confronting a person. 

There is something about these kinds of situations that spurs the mind to mull over what is being perceived and brings to bear memorized information that can assist in meeting what the situation calls for.  It could be a problem, a delight, a curiosity, or an emotionally inducing image.  In all such cases, at least initially, there is a surprise element to it, something a person did not expect.  The point is that whatever such an occurrence substantively contains, the person is willing to move into System 2 thinking which is, by its nature, lazy and unwilling to be activated.  Apparently, System 2 needs prodding to get going.  Generally, it is activated so as to arrive at a satisfying result that “resolves” the situation in question.  That is, it solves the problem, understands the delight, quells the curiosity, or handles the emotion so as to be satisfied with what happens next.  At least, that is what System 2 sets out to accomplish. 

Why is System 2 reluctant?  We know it is tiring to use it.  Studies show that such thinking physically eats up energy (noted by the quantities of sugar that blood carries to the brain when System 2 gets activated).  Therefore, it calls for effort.  It rummages through long-term memory and finds, understands, and figures out how to apply those memories to the challenge presented by the situation at hand.  Sometimes that’s relatively easy to do; at other times it is not. 

People who are good at this sort of thing, who have learned (through instruction and/or experience) to find such thinking enjoyable, and, as a result are good at problem solving or creatively dealing with emotional material, can command, usually, a hefty salary.  We hope our doctor is that sort of person; we hope our political and government leaders are, and we hope our teachers are. 

My experience is, though, that most are not.  But so it goes.  How to rummage and how to enjoy rummaging through our long-term memory are important aspects of education and some educators are conscious of the challenge to fill that memory with important “stuff” and to teach students to diligently, analytically, creatively retrieve and process those memories so they are able to do important and rewarding things in life.



[1] This intrusion into the brain is by the means of scanning devices.

[2] There are evolutionary reasons for this bias.

[3] Kahneman, D.  (2011).  Thinking, fast, and slow.  New York, NY:  Farrar, Straus, and Giroux.

No comments:

Post a Comment