[Note: This posting is the fourth of a series of
postings regarding adolescence. The
reader is invited to click on the previous three postings – and any other
postings – that lead to the content of this one.]
If one doubts the high jinx adolescents are apt to perform or
indulge in, one can just pick up any copy of their local newspaper and read a story
describing mayhem or near mayhem involving a teenager or group of them. It can be a petty crime, some altercation, or
otherwise mischief causing harm to members of the community. Sometimes the acts can be attributed to
drinking, recklessness, irresponsible driving, and/or drug use. All of it, by any consideration, can be
considered risky behavior.
Then there are
those unreported cases but can have long lasting consequences. Of course, common examples are derived from engaging
in unprotected sex. Sex becomes a new
source of intense drive among this age group.
Beyond the incidence of sexually transmitted diseases, many family
stories have been shaped by an unexpected pregnancy and subsequent birth of a
baby out of wedlock or one out of an ill-conceived marriage. Sometimes the results are happy, but
sometimes, even often, they are not.
Unwanted births
often lead to unwanted children or so defenders of abortion rights have
argued. They have cited evidence tying legalized
abortion to a drop in crime rates.[1] This indicates a social cost to unwanted
pregnancies. Surely, this argument –
associating abortion with the drop in crime – can be questioned, but it does
reflect on how what is generally considered a private course of behavior can
have meaningful consequences to a community or a nation. Add to this, the correlated potential of how
unprotective sex has been associated with parental abuse and low rates of
parental support.[2]
And that does not even consider the
personal consequences such as an education being cut short because a mother
takes care of a newborn without the help of the baby’s father. Or how such an event captures what should have
been the rewarding years of a retired grandparent(s) – this writer has seen
this development in more than one household.
Psychologically, one can ask: are adolescents more prone to engage in
risk-taking behavior, as compared to adults?
In line with this question is: are
the cognitive and emotional processes similar or different from adults or are
adolescents simply not as skilled in managing risk?
This last question brings up the argument
that the only difference between adults and say teenagers is the goals and aims
each group strives to attain. The difference
is not what each member of said group attempts to do, but over what he/she is attempting
to accomplish. The difference, it can be
hypothesized, is over the aims not the processes or the physical elements of
the brain.
There is evidence supporting this
hypothesis and it is provided by a study guided by behavioral theory. That is, the study found that youngsters demonstrate
that they give more importance to positive results – rewards – then do adults. Of heightened attraction are rewards one can categorize
as social.[3] But this not a simple matter. Quoting Dustin Albert and Lawrence Steinberg in
the cited article, “[s]tated simply, adolescent decision making is a complex
and multiply [sic] determined phenomena.”[4]
This sort of findings seems to
indicate that there is little or no difference in ways risks are seen and that
the difference lies with what is valued.
Different values lead to different conclusions as to what to do. And if one considers that behavior is the
product of a long evolutionary process – one occupied by surviving in nomadic,
scavenger groupings – this risk-taking proclivity might have served to attract
sought after mates in more primitive settings.[5]
This mate choosing function can be
one of many factors – such as impulsivity, peer presence, ability beliefs, desire
for thrills, peer influences, competitiveness, and personal interests – that
encourage a youngster to take a risk in performing some behavior.[6] These factors can take many forms.
For example, surviving risky
situations adds to some young people’s confidence in their abilities and
letting them and others believe they are ready to leave their parents’
protection or domicile. In a given
group, having this more risk-taking members in their midst can balance out
those who are more conservative; the first willing to try new things and ways
of behaving, the second preserving the derived “wisdom” of what has gone before
a given challenge makes its presence known.
The idea of wisdom will shortly be
revisited, but first a word or two concerning inhibition or the lack of it. Researchers have investigated how adolescents
are apt to fall into undesirable patterns of behavior if they are not continuously
held accountable. Behavioralists call
this extinction learning.
Most people know how the initial
experiments in behavioral study – Pavlov’s using a bell to introduce food to a
dog that has the dog salivate and after repetition having the dog salivate
without the food – but might ask: what
happens after repetition without the food?
Eventually the dog stops salivating.
This is called extinction learning.
Apparently, teenagers are prone to readily ignore what they have learned
especially if the lesson is distasteful.[7]
In turn, when the lesson to be
learned has to do with risk, the inhibitions to performing the unwanted
behavior can more readily reappear. At
times, these behaviors can be associated, for example, with danger, and so a recurrence
of it can have serious consequences.
Behaviors such as unprotected sex or the use of illicit drugs can
readily lead to serious trouble even if the youngster is not initially caught
breaking the rules against such behaviors.[8] The challenge of parenting an adolescence can
seem to never end.
And now a
quick word on wisdom. To begin, wisdom
is not intelligence, at least as intelligence is measured by IQ tests – without
appropriate training, adolescents do not improve, relative to others, on those
tests. But they can improve on their
ability to conjure up insights and exhibit better judgments. Experience does help youngsters in doing
these things better.[9]
And with that
bit of information, this series on adolescence comes to an end. Hopefully, especially if he/she is a civics
teacher, the reader can gain useful information that can be applied when
dealing with that age group. Civic
responsibilities call on a certain level of maturity and immature
characteristic in students can be obstacles in getting those students to learn
and appreciate what goes into being a good citizen.
Oh, as a
parting shot, if a reader who would like to read (or see) a real world example
of what these posting say about maturing or being immature (including extinction
learning), a classic example is conveyed by an episode of Baseball, the
Ken Burns’ series on PBS. That example
is of the life of Babe Ruth. There is a
man who set a slew of records while consuming not performance enhancing drugs,
but consuming performance inhibiting drugs.
He was made for baseball but exhibited repeatedly the types of behaviors
one associates with adolescence.[10]
[1] Robert J. Barro, “Does abortion Lower the Crime
Rate,” Business Week, September 7, 1999, 30, online cite accessed
January 16, 2020, https://scholar.harvard.edu/barro/files/99_0927_crimerate_bw.pdf .
[2] Tom Luster and Stephen A. Small, “Factors Associated
with Sexual Risk Taking Behaviors among Adolescents,” Journal of Marriage and
Family, 46, 3, 622-632, online access to article accessed January 16, 2020, https://www.jstor.org/stable/352873?origin=crossref&seq=1 . This article
goes on to emphasize the occurrence of transmission of sexually transmitted
diseases that have a personal toll and a toll on the community.
[3] Dustin Albert and Lawrence Steinberg, “Judgement and Decision
Making in Adolescence,” Journal of Research on Adolescence, 21, 1,
211-224, online cite accessed January 16, 2020, http://repository.brynmawr.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1053&context=psych_pubs .
[4] Ibid., 4.
[5] Steven Pinker, How the Mind Works (New York,
NY: W. W. Norton and Company, 1997).
[6] David C. Miller and James P. Byrnes, “The Role of
Contextual and Personal Factors in Children’s Risk Taking,” Developing
Psychology, 33, 5, 814-823, online abstract accessed January 16, 2020, https://psycnet.apa.org/doiLanding?doi=10.1037%2F0012-1649.33.5.814 .
[7] Jessica McCullum, Jee Hyun Kim, and Rick Richardson,
“Impaired Extinction Retention in Adolescent Rats: Effects of D-Cycloserine,” Neurosychopharmacolony,
35, 10 (September 2010), 2134-2142, online rendition accessed January 16, 2020,
https://www.nature.com/articles/npp201092 . Yes, the
title refers to rats. Teenagers are not
rats, but this article informs one as follows:
“The developmental trajectory
of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) in both rats and humans is nonlinear, with a
notable decline in synaptic density during adolescence, potentially creating a
‘natural lesion' preparation at this age.”
This condition leads to extinction learning in both adolescents and
rats.
[8] Linda Patia Spear, “Adolescent Neurodevelopment,”
Journal Adolescent Health, 52, 202 (February 2013), online rendition accessed
January 16, 2020, https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3982854/ . Of note,
this type of research (for example, this citation and the previous one) reverts
to neurological factors and that fact diminishes the claim that teenagers and
adults share the same basic brain physical (structural and chemical) elements.
[9] Moisha Pasupathi, Ursula M. Staudinger, and Paul B.
Baltes, “Seeds of Wisdom: Adolescents’
Knowledge and Judgment about Difficult Life Problems,” Development Psychology,
37, 3, 351-361, abstract accessed January 16, 2020, https://psycnet.apa.org/doiLanding?doi=10.1037%2F0012-1649.37.3.351 .
[10] Geoffrey C. Ward and Ken Burns, Baseball: An Illustrated History (New York,
NY: Alfred A. Knopf, 1994) AND Ken Burns
and Lynn Novick, Baseball: A Film by
Ken Burns, PBS Production, 1994.
No comments:
Post a Comment