In terms of the
construct, liberated federalism, the first item of interest to this blogger is
the assumptions regarding decision making – that would be decision making in
the realm of governance and politics.
And in this vein, the duality mentioned in the last posting between
forces that either focus political concerns or studies at local communal levels
or at national levels will be highlighted shortly in an upcoming posting.
But before addressing
this duality directly, however, some contextualizing needs to be done. Specifically, factors of decision-making
processes need to be fleshed out. While
this posting will only skim the surface of this topic, one might find the path
it paves a bit challenging – hopefully readers will enjoy the scenery.
This synthesis of views –
which this blog is currently addressing – between natural rights and critical theory
is not totally divorced from the nation’s earlier view, parochial federalism,
with its more local orientation. That
orientation stands in contrast with a more nationalistic orientation that the natural
rights view assumes, and which is prominent today. Of note, the natural rights view relies
heavily on assumptions posed by behavioral sciences and, with that, an
important link to decision making becomes relevant.
Within that behavioral mode
of thought, it is worth noting that most decisions that people make are made to
advance one’s interests on the margin and the assumption held here is that
reality is more complex and that decisions are based on several cognitive
domains not adequately addressed by behaviorist analysis. Therefore, this aspect of natural rights is
judged to be wanting.
While it is beyond the purpose
of this account to present a comprehensive model for decision making, the
process of making decisions is seen as one in which individuals weigh several
motivating factors at any given time.[1] And so, before explaining the assumptions
entailed here with this synthesis, the importance of decision making should be
established and somewhat explained.
In terms of considering
mental constructs that pertain to guiding how citizens view governance and
politics, the question of how and why people decide to act politically as they
do seems to be of central importance. Decision
making is a precursor to all actions excluding actions that are classified as
reflex reactions. That is, before people
act, they decide to act, i.e., they are motivated to individually act in a certain
fashion. In turn, people’s motivations
vary in terms of intensity and direction.[2]
Therefore, to get a clear
understanding of why people act the way they do, one needs to have an
explanation of why they have decided to act in a particular fashion; why they
have generated the motivation to act in the ways they do. What follows is how this blogger has broken
down this process – part relies heavily on psychological works of well-regarded
scholars and part on self-reflection.
To assume people are
merely responding to the conditions facing them in such ways as to marginally advance
their interests, as crude behaviorists do, leads one to miscalculate various
aspects of those decisions. That is, it
might very well underestimate how these actors weigh the values before them or
the priorities these actors place on those values, not to mention the personal histories
of similar situations people bring to that point in time.[3]
Granted, a lot of this happens at the
subconscious level, but it happens, nonetheless.
Those who study these decision-making
sessions write about these miscalculations by those who engage in the politics
of various challenging situations; for example, the politics of ethnically
diverse nations and how “irrational” behaviors there can and at times are
judged to be. Usually, such studies look
at segments of those nations’ populations and have often judged them to be chaotic.
In those cases, which are
often like other sorts of decision-making episodes in challenging situations, the
chaos originates not from what the minds read to be the facts of the situations
in question. Instead, people misread how
their values are engaged and/or miscalculate the strength of their or of
others’ values as they pertain to the situations at hand.
And this description of
what is going on is not just the opinion of unsophisticated parties but of
experts who find it excessively difficult to predict the behaviors of those
populations. What is called for are
closer views and understandings both of what mental processes go into decision
making and of what such decisions potentially consider; that can be, as hinted
above, a large array of complicated notions, beliefs, habits, desires, fears,
and complicated memories.
It turns out, therefore,
that there are numerous factors that go into these acts of deciding to act. One, as the behaviorists argue, are the
conditions of the present situation, or at least, what is perceived from the
current situation or from extrinsic factors.[4] For purposes of identification in this
discussion, this set of cognitive elements can be called the real domain. The real domain is constituted by the
theories or paradigms one holds about reality, one’s memories, and one’s current
perceptions.
These elements make
themselves usually known in a holistic way in that individuals attempt to
create congruence among their substantive components. Usually upon reflection, often in response to
a relevant question, people think in terms of individual elements. Short of that, the sum of these elements constitutes
a holistic picture of what is.
Components or elements that
do not “fit” substantively; they offend the logical wholeness of the rest and
cause internal discomfort known as dissonance.[5] In this domain there are the recognitions of
all the relevant values and goals people have in life as matters of fact. These elements – while in themselves belonging
to the ideal domain (to be explained below) – add vibrance to any dissonance
one might feel in each situation. It can
intensify dissonance and when it is experienced, individuals seek and usually
arrive at some accommodation so that the dissonance is lessened.
How? For one, individuals can abandon their related
values or goals or rationalize the perceived inconsistencies in question if the
situations allow. Another option is that
they can neglect the inconsistencies, or the affected values or goals can be
diminished, if only for the time being, in importance. This last possibility is often an important attribute
of that other domain, the aforementioned ideal domain – that is, those beliefs
individuals hold concerning what should or should not be.[6]
In that other ideal mental
domain, people harbor those values and goals in some order of priority which is
to some degree (usually when they are fairly high in intensity or in relation
to other values and goals) are readily recognized and felt by the individuals
in question.[7]
Priority listings are
supported by emotions and feelings, which in turn are motivating forces within
the perceived mental purviews of individuals.
The substantive elements of the ideal domain are products of decisions
themselves. But unlike the decisions to
act, these decisions are made without the consequences of such decisions immediately
present. One can, in a moment of reflection,
decide to hold in high regard the desire to do some activity but when the
opportunity arises, one can think better of that initial decision.
Ideal decision making is
done upon reflection of past experiences, socialized lessons, and formed habits. Most of the literature discusses the
formulation of such ideals as those which are formed because of past rewards
and punishments. Some have commented on
the role communal settings have played in the formation of ideals and often take
on moralistic status and can be described as duties and obligations.
And usually, the elements
of ideals conform with the way they were developed or attained. For example, technical values are generally
acquired in technical environments; loving values tend to be attained in loving
relationships. The substance of the
values and goals not only tend to be congruent with each other, but also with
their formulation.
That is, they create
patterns of dealing with the entailed emotions, the allegiance to sets of mores
– this harkens to that delightful book, Habits of the Heart, by Robert Bellah,
et al.[8]
– and from the first domain, the real domain, their supportive beliefs – the “habits
of the mind.” All this is sensed not as
individual components, but with a comfortable, repeatable wholeness within one’s
consciousness – “this is just the way I see things,” or “this is just the way I
feel about it.”
Another set of
motivations or influences on the decision-making process is the physiological
factors. Here, the elements of this set
of factors are not intentional on the part of people. The elements include the genetic makeup of
individuals, the changes, through aging, in the chemical compositions of
people, and the social reactions to behaviors that such genetic and chemical
compositions encourage.
These factors or
motivations are not chosen by individuals but are the product of given
conditions that are present in either affected situations that people confront
or exist more or less chronically in their biological makeups.[9] Of course the other factor that affects this
domain is accidental occurrences that have negative physical consequences –
from a slip and fall to wartime injuries.
People so afflicted may have had life changing experiences that have
affected how they see their worlds, which include mental effects on their real
and ideal domains.
So, this account will
stop here and simply state that decision making sessions are influenced by what
occupies people’s minds in the form of what they “see” as the real (either in the
present situations or from what they recollect from their past), what they
believe should or should not be – their ideal beliefs – and what the mind
recognizes are the physiological conditions or limitations that people’s bodies
are experiencing. In the next posting, this account will venture into the time
space in which decisions are made.
[1] For a lighthearted description of this claim, see “Critical
Thinking and Decision-Making: Why Is It
So Hard to Make Decisions?” GCF Global (n.d.), accessed June 10, 2023, https://edu.gcfglobal.org/en/problem-solving-and-decision-making/why-is-it-so-hard-to-make-decisions/1/#
AND for a more professional account (involving business related decisions) see
Vassundhara Sawhney, “Why Do We Try to Dodge Difficult Decisions?” Ascend
(August 19, 2021), accessed June 10, 2023, https://hbr.org/2021/08/why-do-we-try-to-dodge-difficult-decisions AND for a more scholarly account, a bit dated, Steven
Pinker, How the Mind Works (New York, NY: W. W. Norton and Company, 1997).
[2] Robert Slavin, Educational Psychology: Theory and Practice (London,
England: Pearson, 2022).
[3] This has been studied from various perspectives. For example, how others influence people’s
decision making, see Vanessa K. Bohn, M. Mhdi Roghanizad, and Amy Z. Xu,
“Underestimating Our Influence over Others’ Unethical Behavior and Decisions,” Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40, 3, accessed June 10, 2023, https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/0146167213511825 AND for a more standard account, see Daniel Patrick
Moynihan, Pandaemonium: Ethnicity in
International Politics, (New York, NY:
Oxford University Press, 1993).
[4] Slavin, Educational Psychology.
[5] Ibid.
[6] Ibid.
[7] Robert L. Solso, Cognitive Psychology (8th
Edition), (Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon,
2007).
[8] Robert N. Bellah, Richard Madsen, William M.
Sullivan, Ann Swidler, and Steven M. Tipton, Habits of the Heart: Individualism and Commitment in American Life
(New York, NY: Harper and Row,
Publishers, 1985/2007).
[9] For example, Bruce Goldman, “Two Minds: The Cognitive Differences between Men and
Women,” Stanford Medicine Magazine (May 22, 2017) accessed June 10,
2023, https://www.google.com/search?q=recent+review+of+moir+and+jessel+brain+sex&rlz=1C1RXMK_enUS966US966&oq=recent+review+of+moir+and+jessel+brain+sex&aqs=chrome..69i57.20198j1j7&sourceid=chrome&ie=UTF-8#ip=1 AND Anne Moir and David Jessel, Brain Sex: The Real Difference between Men and Women
(New York, NY: Dell Publishing, 1989).
No comments:
Post a Comment