A Crucial Element of Democracy

This is a blog by Robert Gutierrez ...
While often taken for granted, civics education plays a crucial role in a democracy like ours. This Blog is dedicated to enticing its readers into taking an active role in the formulation of the civics curriculum found in their local schools. In order to do this, the Blog is offering a newer way to look at civics education, a newer construct - liberated federalism or federation theory. Daniel Elazar defines federalism as "the mode of political organization that unites separate polities within an overarching political system by distributing power among general and constituent governments in a manner designed to protect the existence and authority of both." It depends on its citizens acting in certain ways which Elazar calls federalism's processes. Federation theory, as applied to civics curriculum, has a set of aims. They are:
*Teach a view of government as a supra federated institution of society in which collective interests of the commonwealth are protected and advanced.
*Teach the philosophical basis of government's role as guardian of the grand partnership of citizens at both levels of individuals and associations of political and social intercourse.
*Convey the need of government to engender levels of support promoting a general sense of obligation and duty toward agreed upon goals and processes aimed at advancing the common betterment.
*Establish and justify a political morality which includes a process to assess whether that morality meets the needs of changing times while holding true to federalist values.
*Emphasize the integrity of the individual both in terms of liberty and equity in which each citizen is a member of a compacted arrangement and whose role is legally, politically, and socially congruent with the spirit of the Bill of Rights.
*Find a balance between a respect for national expertise and an encouragement of local, unsophisticated participation in policy decision-making and implementation.
Your input, as to the content of this Blog, is encouraged through this Blog directly or the Blog's email address: gravitascivics@gmail.com .
NOTE: This blog has led to the publication of a book. The title of that book is TOWARD A FEDERATED NATION: IMPLEMENTING NATIONAL CIVICS STANDARDS and it is available through Amazon in both ebook and paperback versions.

Friday, January 10, 2020

HOMEMADE MODELS


[Note:  This posting is the second of a series of posting regarding adolescence.  The reader is invited to click on the previous posting – and any other postings – that leads to the content of this one.]
Probably most important, when one considers how secondary students – made up of adolescents – approach social studies and, more specifically, civics, is their ability to think abstractly at a much higher level as compared to younger children.  This, given the goals and aims of a federalist guided curriculum and given how this thinking operates is both good and bad.
Or better stated, it both allows instructors to pursue and explain the complexities of the social world to a high degree of sophistication but also deal with students who rely on any “theories” their thinking produces with a lack of questioning.  This two-faced challenge needs an explanation.
          As to the cognitive development of this change, knowledge of it in psychological literature can be traced to the work of Jean Piaget.  His and subsequent studies in the field identified that the ability of young people to think abstractly is highly dependent on them acquiring the facility to think deductively.[1]  In turn, once deduction is possible, a person can engage in planning the future, anticipate consequences to actions, and think of optional outcomes and explanations about what has happened or what will happen.
          Depending on how good they become at these mental skills, they can take effective roles in debates.  Suddenly parents find their adolescent off-springs questioning their, the parents’, assumptions or judgements concerning probabilities; for example, “if you don’t do what I tell you, you will suffer (blank) consequence.”  Well, “maybe not” can suddenly be the response. 
These young people discover figurative language – puns, metaphors, and analogies – and can decipher it.  They might even use sarcasm.  Language becomes more of a tool beyond just communicating observable facts or feelings.  With that tool, they can more readily apply higher level reasoning in which they can question social, political, and economic claims. 
They can also understand, appreciate, and support the use of values or ideological precepts.  But they are hindered by both a lack of experience in their attempts to make sense of the world and a lack of dispositional sentiments to learn from that experience.  That is, held beliefs or attitudes are not easily dismissed or changed.
One can deduct from these more general capabilities adolescents being able to form and test hypotheses.  If one thinks about it, a hypothesis demands a person to look at some problem, abstract from it those factors that either create, support, or accentuate the problem, and then test that judgement in a “if/then” supposition against reality.  And in this, one finds how central these skills are in students being able to inquire about civic problems or other social conditions of interest. 
But here is the rub; along with these skills, a youngster is subject to prejudicial thinking – the dispositional bias just mentioned.  And this possibility leads those who study cognitive development to various theoretical approaches.  In one approach[2] this duality is highlighted by pointing out the positive and negative turns these acquired capacities entail. 
In accordance to that approach, one can describe the above mental process as a mental simulation of reality.  And a lot goes into that thinking on the part of a subject including what one wants reality to be.  That is, one can call that simulation the function of biases in which one hypothesizes not to test a preference but to confirm a bias regardless of its veracity. 
This can undermine the validity of those mental processes.  This takes various forms depending on how formal the test of a hypothesis is.  Sometimes people test hypotheses very informally in day-to-day life; people just dismiss this as normal thinking or conversation.  In those cases, one readily sees biased thinking demonstrated by what people say about their dealings with common problems. 
But there are those who go about this quite formally – scientists and other scholars.  And yet, even with them one can find cases where biases are confirmed or assumed without enough grounds.  Or, as Jonathan St. B. T. Evans reports, they agree with a fallacious argument in their deductive reasoning. 
For example, ungrounded biases can infect statistical analysis at any stage in which statistics are used (in designing a study, in collecting the data, in analyzing the numbers, or in reporting the “findings”).[3]  Consequently, strict protocols are used to minimize the effects of any biases the scholar might bring to his/her efforts.  Apparently, the motive to find what one wants to find is a strong motivation indeed.
So, if that describes what professionals – who are paid to be objective and have acquired the education and training to perform objectively – worry about, what can one expect from adolescents?  One should remember that most of what young people know has come from experiences; what one can call heuristic – hands-on – experiences.  And reflecting upon this state, a reputable model is often cited in the literature.
Again, citing Evans,[4] one can utilize a heuristic-analytic theory or reasoning.  That model identifies three principles.  The first is that a person, in his/her reasoning and accompanying judgements, relies on epistemic (degree of validation) mental models.  They reflect take-aways from singular experiences a person encounters.  They affect a person’s thinking process at a preconscious mental level.  They function to contextualize subsequent relevant challenges. 
How?  These mental biases heighten simultaneously felt goals.  This is the second principle.  Yes, the individual can evaluate a held model or explanation, but he/she is prone to accept it and defend it to him/herself and to others even when it is being challenged.  It reminds one of how parents protect their own; that is, he/she gave “birth” to the biased explanation.
But then there are experiences that more seriously challenge the held model and if the immediate realities are sufficiently incongruent with the model, the person can potentially reject it.  This judgmental process constitutes principle three.  These challenging events can simply present themselves to a person or they can be programed by others such as teachers and their planned lesson experiences.  Of course, this can be of varying degrees of importance to a student. 
This writer can report he has gone through two major, life-changing (transforming) experiences or set of events in his life – the first when he was nineteen.  They can be monumental to one’s sense of self and can prove to be quite the determining factor in subsequent decisions as to how one defines personal relationships or job/career seeking decisions.  Sometimes they prove to be beneficial, sometimes they don’t.
So, the three principles are the singularity principle (from where models are generated), the relevancy principle (the relation to personal goals), and the satisficing principle (the judgement over a model’s functionality).  Evan’s abstract states,
At a minimum, analytic processing of models is required so as to generate inferences or judgements relevant to the task instructions, but more active intervention may result in modification or replacement of default models generated by the heuristic system.  Evidence for this theory is provided by a review of a wide range of literature on thinking and reasoning.[5]
All this indicates that adolescents are open to instruction that can call on students to recognize the mental models they have formulated, evaluate those models, and either modify or reject those models.  This in not to say that they are predisposed to do so; they harbor allegiance to the models they have created.
          At a more practical level, one should not see these models as totally dysfunctional.  They do serve as “advance organizers” that assist the young person – or anyone – to categorize and be able to place in priority stimuli as they become apparent.  At a very basic level, this is essential.  The question becomes:  can one dislodge harmful advanced organizers?[6]  Apparently, they, the students, can and that, when one reflects, is a basic aim of education, and, when the topic is social, is the aim of social studies.


[1] Gertrude G. Zeinstra, Maria A. Koelen, Frnas J. Kok, and Cees deGraaf, “Cognitive Development and Children’s Perception of Fruit and Vegetables; A Qualitative Study, International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 4, 30 (2007), online version accessed January 8, 2020, https://ijbnpa.biomedcentral.com/articles/10.1186/1479-5868-4-30 AND Kendra Cherry, The Formal Operational Stage of Cognitive Development, Very Well Mind, June 24, 2019, accessed January 8, 2020, https://www.verywellmind.com/formal-operational-stage-of-cognitive-development-2795459 .  This latter cite is a popular version of the former.

[2] In regards to the effect of biases on hypothesis testing see Jonathan S. B. T. Evans, “Hypothetical Thinking:  Dual Processes in Reasoning and Judgement,”  Psychology Press, 2007, abstract accessed January 8, 2020, https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2007-02337-000 .

[3] In regards to the effect of biases on hypothesis testing see Jonathan S. B. T. Evans, “Hypothetical Thinking:  Dual Processes in Reasoning and Judgement,”  Psychology Press, 2007, abstract accessed January 8, 2020, https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2007-02337-000 .

[4] Jonathan S. B. T. Evans, “The Heuristic-Analytic Theory of Reasoning:  Extension and Evaluation,” Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13, 2006, 378-395, abstract accessed January 9, 2020, https://www.researchgate.net/publication/6746428_The_Heuristic-Analytic_Theory_of_Reasoning_Extension_and_Evaluation .

[5] Ibid.

[6] A subsequent important question is:  what constitutes harm?

Tuesday, January 7, 2020

GETTING TO KNOW


Let there be no doubt, all secondary teachers benefit from the knowledge and beliefs shared by biologists and social scientists concerning adolescence.  That is, they, those teachers, should investigate what these scholars have discovered and speculated over concerning the span of years lodged between childhood and adulthood.  But no field of instruction can benefit more from such investigation than social studies and particularly civics education.
          This blog has in the past addressed adolescence and, with this posting, will begin a renewed reporting as to what the field of psychology and other fields have found.  One place to begin is cognitive development.  The cognitive capacities of young people from the ages of twelve to twenty-one increase significantly.[1]  Physical studies, including magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies, of the brain support this claim as one can detect the changes of the prefrontal cortex and how it functions regarding blood flows and other bodily processes.[2]
          Derived from such research, certain researchers and theorists have developed the dual systems model.  That model strives to explain various observable behavior patterns one can associate with adolescence.  Namely, the model focuses on risk-taking.  It posits that those behaviors result from a higher degree of sensitivity for rewards and a less than optimal level of impulse control and further, these patterns are “encouraged” by biological factors.[3]
          With that very brief overview of the biological, one can judge how productive more theoretical thinking has been concerning these young people’s changing character.  Beginning with Jean Piaget, prominent psychologists have added to what has become the current state of understanding regarding these perplexing years.  From cognitive development to emotional development, a rich literature exists. [4] 
          Psychological studies of cognitive development have centered on various aspects of that development.  They include attention,[5] memory,[6] pace of thinking,[7] the ability to organize thinking,[8] and the ability to reflect on one’s own thinking patterns.  This last topic will be emphasized in future postings.
          As for this posting, the purpose is to introduce the reader to the various concerns one can associate with adolescence.  This should be of interest to those who are charged with instructing mostly teenagers about civic matters.  It is felt here that such information for those educators can be relevant and useful. 
This posting is but the first of a series that will further develop this effort.  It contains some initial samples of a literature teachers can delve into to further strengthen the choices he/she makes in the designing and delivery of lessons.  In short, this series will aim to further enrich what this blog has already shared in this field of interest.
          The next posting will highlight the development of hypothetical and abstract thinking.  Useful is a familiarity with the concept, structure of knowledge.  Here is what this blog shared about this concept in a previous posting,
Knowledge, that is the knowing of truth, has been described as a structured, mental content.  That structure, commonly referred to as the structure of knowledge, has occupied the attention of some of the most prominent thinkers in the field of education.  That includes Jean Piaget, Lev Vygotski, and Jerome Bruner.  A more recent educator is H. Lynn Erickson.
A common description of this structure includes, in an order from the most concrete to the most abstract, the following:  facts, concepts, [topics,] principles or generalizations, and theory.  This progression indicates that facts, logically associated, form concepts; concepts form principles or generalizations, and, one can guess, principles or generalizations form theories.[9] 
One can add to this last category models or hypothesized explanations.  The reader, by reading this quote, has done his/her homework. 
or just “Google” structure of knowledge for various visual representations of how humans organize what they know.


[1] Steven R. Smith and Leonard Handler, The Clinical Assessment of Children and Adolescents:  A Practitioner’s Handbook (New York, NY:  Routledge, 2007).  An overview of this book can be found online; see https://www.taylorfrancis.com/books/9781315831473 , accessed January 5, 2020.
[2] Suparna Choudhury, Sarah-Jayne Blakemore, and Tony Charman, “Social Cognitive Development during Adolescence,” Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1, 3 (December 2006), 165-174, accessed January 5, 2020, https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2555426/ .
[3] B. J. Casey, Rebecca M. Jones, and Leah H. Somerville, “Braking and Accelerating of the Adolescent Brain, Journal of Research on Adolescence, 21, 1 (March 1, 2011), 21-33, accessed January 5, 2020, https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3070306/ .
[4] An instructive review of the various theoretical approaches to this field of interest (behavioral, information processing, and constructivist), can be found in the textbook, Educational Psychology:  Theory and Practice.  See Robert Slavin, Educational Psychology:  Theory and Practice (12th Edition) (Boston, MA:  Allyn and Bacon, 2018).
[5] For example, Andrew R. Schiff and Irwin J. Knopf, “The Effect of Task Demands on Attention Allocation in Children of Different Ages,” Child Development (JSTOR), 56, 3 (June 1985), 621-630.  This study compares attention capacity between 9-year-olds and 13-year-olds.
[6]  For example, Daniel P. Keating, “Cognitive and Brain Development,” in Handbook of Adolescent Psychology, eds. R. M. Lerner and L. Steinberg (Portsmouth, NH:  Heinemann, 2004), 45-84.  This work found association between reasoning and memory.
[7] For example, Robert V. Kail and Emilio Ferrer, “Processing Speed in Childhood and Adolescence:  Longitudinal Models for Examining Developmental Change,” Child Development (SRCD), November 3, 2007, abstract accessed January 6, 2020, https://srcd.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01088.x .  This is a review study of various longitudinal models that purport to describe change in processing speed among children and adolescents.
[8] For example, Ann L. Brown, “The Development of Memory:  Knowing, Knowing about Knowing, and Knowing How to Know,” Advances in Child Development and Behavior, a series of books, Volume 10 (1975), 103-152, abstract accessed January 6, 2020, https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0065240708600099?via%3Dihub .  The focus of this work is to look at how memory affects self-reflection over what is known by the individual child and adolescent.
[9] Robert Gutierrez, “It’s Not All Good or Bad,” Gravitas:  A Voice for Civics, October 24, 2017, accessed January 6, 2020, https://gravitascivics.blogspot.com/2017/10/its-not-all-good-or-bad.html .